home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- Perfect Pitch v1.2 (c) 1987 W.P. Kraslawsky
- Music Ear Training All Rights Reserved
-
-
-
- I. Introduction
-
- This program is designed to provide drills leading to the development of
- perfect pitch recognition. "Perfect pitch recognition" is defined in
- this program as the ability to hear any pitch and identify it by name
- (A, A-flat, F-sharp, etc.), especially without using a previously
- defined reference pitch. A related ability, "perfect pitch recall," is
- the ability to hum or sing a pitch on request, without reference to a
- previously defined reference pitch. Recognition is a prerequisite to
- recall. Only recognition is addressed by this program.
-
- The user is immediately cautioned not to expect results without effort.
- I have had sufficient success to want to share it with others. However,
- much patience and practice is going to be necessary before consistent
- results are achieved.
-
- This program is based on concepts presented in the handbook, "Perfect
- Pitch: Color-Hearing for Expanded Musical Awareness" by David L.
- Burge. If you use this program and would like to pursue the topic in
- greater detail, then look for books by D.L. Burge. Any bookstore such
- as B.Dalton or Waldenbooks can assist you via their Books-in-Print
- catalog. A course using workbooks and tapes by D.L. Burge is advertised
- in various music magazines such as "Keyboard."
-
-
-
-
- II. Concepts
-
- The concept of color-hearing can be understood by considering an
- analogy. We are all familiar with the difference between black-and-
- white (B/W) vs. color TV, movies, and photographs. The essence of the
- analogy is that low-to-high pitches in music are similar to low-to-high
- greyness in a B/W movie or photo.
-
- Although we can obtain great pleasure from a B/W movie, there is much
- information and richness which is only available using color vision.
- Similarly, we can obtain great pleasure from music heard only in terms
- of combinations of low and high pitches, but full color-hearing adds
- detail and richness. Burge, an accomplished musician, composer and
- instructor, expresses his perfect-pitch abilities in terms of this
- analogy.
-
- Examination of a B/W photo will show many variations of grey, depending
- on the color of the original real-life object. Given a chart with side-
- by-side comparisons of grey vs. original color, there is a chance for
- identifying the original color of a grey patch in a B/W photo. However,
- this is not the thrust of the training method. Instead, we observe that
- each color has an intrinsic quality. You can recognize something as
- being "red", without having to compare it to something "green." Your
- recognition of the color red is based on experience, not on comparisons.
- In particular, you do not think of red as being more grey or less grey
- than green. Dark reds and bright reds share the quality we recognize as
- redness, and that quality is different from the common quality of
- greenness shared by dark greens and bright greens.
-
- Similarly, each musical pitch has an intrinsic quality which can be
- recognized with experience. Although pitches differ from each other in
- terms of highness or lowness on the frequency scale, this is not
- relevant to its intrinsic quality. An F-sharp (F#) has a different
- quality from a C. And, all F#'s on a piano keyboard share the same
- quality, which is different from the quality shared by all C's. Unlike
- our childhood education in visual colors, however, we have not received
- childhood training in these auditory qualities.
-
- The task of pitch-color recognition is made more difficult by the lack
- of an appropriate vocabulary. It is as though nobody had ever invented
- the words "green" and "yellow", but we were required to describe the
- difference between a lemon and a lime. Words such as warm, soft, and
- mellow abound in the description of music, but are not consistently
- used. In spite of a lack of consistent descriptive terms available to
- the rest of us, these terms are specifically implied in the study of
- perfect pitch. However, it is not necessary to invent names for the
- intrinsic qualities of the pitches. You will realize that the names of
- some audible qualities are "A-sharp" and "B-flat," just as the names of
- some intrinsic visual qualities are "red" and "green."
-
- This point can be made more clear by reconsidering the visual spectrum.
- Any graphic artist will tell you that red and orange are warm colors,
- and that green and blue are cool colors. You may even appreciate and
- agree with these designations. Suppose, however, that we were taught
- as children to identify the colors of a rainbow as "Warmest," "Warmer,"
- "Warm," "Cool," "Cooler," and "Coolest." We might then find graphic
- artists saying "This degree of warmness is an R," and "That degree of
- coolness is a V," and, "The visual scale is R-O-Y-G-B-V." The graphic
- artist would then produce a composition containing "O-flats" (Orange-
- Red) and "Y-sharps" (Yellow-Green).
-
- Similarly, you find music artists with perfect pitch saying "This degree
- of mellowness is an E-flat," and, "That degree of vibrancy is an F-
- sharp." The musical compositions then include splashes of "E-flats" and
- "F-sharps." Your objective is to learn to recognize the qualities of
- the various pitches. This learning process specifically requires
- associating the perceived quality with the names of the musical notes:
- "A," "A-sharp," and so on. The result will be the ability to perceive
- musical compositions in a new light. The audible difference should be
- as dramatic as the visual difference between a B/W movie and color
- movie.
-
-
-
-
-
- III. Method
-
- I follow the method of D.L. Burge, wherein he compares two pitches he
- considers to be very different: E-flat (Eb) and F-sharp (F#). If all
- of the Eb's and F#'s on a piano keyboard are played at random, one at a
- time, you will begin to recognize a difference. The Eb's tend to be
- softer and more mellow, while the F#'s tend to be more vibrant or
- "twangy." If I tried to express this difference as I perceive it, I
- might do so by singing all Eb's as OOOOO's and all F#'s as EEEEE's.
-
- The method of training is therefore to study these two pitches until the
- differences are mastered, and then to add one pitch at a time. After
- only a short period of time (an hour or two), it is reasonable to expect
- 100% recognition of Eb's vs. F#'s across several octaves. After a
- significant number of consecutive correct guesses, you are ready to add
- another pitch. For example, you might add the next pitch after 100
- consecutive correct identifications.
-
- Three pitches are much more difficult to keep straight than two pitches.
- You can expect to take much longer to learn them. It will take hours
- before you will achieve 100 consecutive correct identifications on the
- three-pitch drill. After that, four-pitch recognition will be a major
- hurdle. Each of the first four pitches chosen by D.L. Burge are
- separated from its neighbors by three semitones (three piano keys
- including white and black). At this point, the test becomes
- symmetrical. All hints based on relative distance between pitches are
- lost. You are completely dependent on recognizing the unique qualities
- of each pitch.
-
- The full sequence of twelve pitches to be learned is described in
- Burge's handbook: Eb, F#, A, C, C#, E, G, Bb, D, F, Ab, and B. After
- that, Burge's method requires naming the pitches within a chord. If you
- reach this point, you will require a multi-voice instrument to proceed.
- This is beyond the capabilities of an IBM PC and is therefore not
- implemented in this program.
-
-
-
- IV. Implementation
-
- The current version of this program provides study and testing drills
- which play pitches for you over four octaves. You may set your own
- pace within a session by increasing ">" or decreasing "<" the number of
- pitches at any time. The number of guesses may be reinitialized at any
- time by pressing "Z" to Zero the counters. No attempt is made to record
- or interpret your scores.
-
- The twelve pitches are shown horizontally and vertically on the full
- screen. You select one of the pitches by entering one of the
- characters, "1234567890-=" as shown in the menu. During the
- recognition test, a tally is maintained of your guesses vs. the correct
- pitches. A perfect score is a perfect diagonal tally. This is more
- easily seen than explained, and will become immediately obvious as soon
- as you begin. The purpose of the tally is to show you which pitches are
- being confused with each other. You can then use the information to
- tailor your study time.
-
- The program provides two basic operational modes. The primary mode is
- the drill, where you test your ability to recognize notes. The
- secondary mode allows you to study and compare notes without testing.
- These are accessed via the "T" and "S" menu items. Under the "T" mode,
- you are presented with a random series of pitches to identify. Under
- the "S" mode, you may selectively listen to different octaves of one
- pitch by pressing the appropriate "1234567890-=" selection. Or, you
- may repeatedly press "S" to obtain a series of random pitches whose
- names are shown immediately. You may rapidly alternate "T" and "S" to
- drill yourself without keeping score, and you may skip pitches by
- repeatedly selecting "T" until you hear one you are sure of. There is
- never a penalty for skipping or for showing a pitch without guessing.
-
- The program is designed to provide continuous Testing or Showing. It
- never takes more that a single key stroke to show the current pitch, to
- show a new pitch, to answer the current pitch and simultaneously receive
- a new pitch, and to obtain and hear the result of a range change (adding
- or removing a pitch from the drill). In order to give yourself (and the
- PC speaker) a break, you may pause the program by entering a "P." The
- program will resume with the same pitch with another "P," or will play a
- new pitch on a different key stroke.
-
- The program also keeps score for you. The percent of correct identifi-
- cations, the count of correct vs. total guesses, and the count of
- consecutive correct guesses are continuously displayed. The selection
- keys, "1234567890-=," were chosen so you can place a labeled strip of
- paper along the top row of your keyboard. That way, you can select your
- answers from the relabeled keys without memorizing the keyboard-pitch
- relationship. All of these features are designed to minimize the
- computer interface. You may concentrate on development of perfect pitch
- recognition without interference from cumbersome command structures.
-
- Your objective is to instantly recognize a pitch by its intrinsic
- quality and to instantly express that recognition by naming it as "A,"
- "A-sharp," "B-flat," and so on. Although you are required to type 1, 2,
- 3, etc., you should always think Eb, F#, A, etc. Placing a strip of
- paper with the pitch names along the top row is highly recommended. In
- time, you should be able to recognize pitches perfectly.
-
-
-
-
-
- V. Shareware
-
- Although my understanding of color-hearing is based on the handbook by
- David L. Burge, this program and documentation are my original
- expressions of that idea. As such, they are protected under copyright
- law.
-
- This program is not Public Domain. It is Shareware. Some shareware
- authors ask for a donation if you like their program. Such donations
- are often only suggested amounts, where the user decides how much it is
- worth. Another type of shareware only provides free trials. If you
- don't like it or don't want to pay for it, then you just don't use it.
- The advantage of this type of shareware program is that you get to try
- it before you buy it.
-
- This program falls in the latter category. I believe that many people
- don't think they can develop perfect pitch. They would not spend money
- on such a far-fetched idea, even if they would really like to develop
- this skill. So here is the deal. This program is for sale, just like
- 1-2-3 and dBASE III are for sale. But, you get to try this program for
- free. If it doesn't work for you, just delete it. If it works for you,
- (and I believe it will), then decide whether it worth the money. If it
- is too expensive, then delete it.
-
- That's where the honesty factor comes in. If you use the program to
- develop real perfect pitch skills, and don't pay me, then you are
- stealing my work. That would be no different from using a copy of 1-2-
- 3 and not paying for it. Or sneaking into a movie without paying. To
- make it easy on you, I have set up some pretty tough standards (music-
- wise). If the program doesn't help you beat those standards, then I
- don't expect any money from you. On the other hand, if you do meet the
- standards, then you got some very heavy use out of my program. If you
- meet the standards and want to continue with future use, you will find
- the following fee schedule very easy to swallow. My program is small
- and limited, so I don't charge much for its use:
-
- A. Continued use after 100 consecutive $ 1.00
- correct id's on two-pitch drill.
-
- B. Continued use after 200 consecutive $ 9.00
- correct id's on four-pitch drill. ----------
- Total $ 10.00
-
- Payment is strictly on the honor system. You are expected to send me $1
- after meeting standard A, BEFORE you continue to work towards standard
- B. When you reach standard B the final $9 fee is due, BEFORE continuing
- to higher levels of development. Working on standard B before mailing
- the first $1 fee is specifically a violation of my right to compensation
- for my work.
-
- There are no hidden files, tricks, traps, or other enforcement methods.
- The program will pause briefly with a congratulatory reminder message
- when you hit these standards. You and your conscience are on your own
- after that.
-
- W.P. Kraslawsky
- 2025 Sarazen Place
- Reston, VA 22091
-
-
-
-
-
- VI. Distribution
-
- Please share this program by giving copies to friends and by uploading
- it to bulletin boards. Even if you do not find this program useful,
- there may be others who would. You play an important part in the
- distribution chain of User-Supported software. Thank you in advance for
- your help.
-
-
- Please observe these minor restrictions on distribution:
-
- (1) It must be included in a library containing other Public
- Domain and User-Supported Shareware programs.
-
- (2) It must be listed as "Perfect Pitch v1.2 Shareware Ear Training"
- as far as possible in the space allowed by the library catalog.
-
- (3) It must be a copy of PITCH12.ARC exactly as you originally
- received it. Please do not modify, rename, or recreate it.
-
- (4) Bulletin board downloading fees must be limited to standard
- charges common to all Public Domain and Sharware programs.
-
- (5) Disk distribution copying fees must be limited to standard
- charges common to all Public Domain and Shareware disks.
-
- (6) For-profit companies which meet the above restrictions must
- obtain my written consent prior to advertising/distribution.
-
-
-
- VII. Update History
-
- v1.1 - 9/27/87
- Documentation has been rewritten.
- "S" function added to switch to Show mode.
- "T" function added to switch to Test mode.
- "P" function added to play/pause speaker. Replaces Space.
- "1234567890-=" under "S" mode added to study pitches.
- Unused keyboard entries are now ignored.
- Menu and other screen items are highlighted and improved.
-
- v1.2 - 9/27/87
- Improved opening instruction screen.
-